The pattern behind self-deception - Michael Shermer
[Music] [Applause] So since I was here last in '06, we discovered that global climate change is turning out to be a pretty serious issue. So we covered that fairly extensively in Skeptic magazine. We investigate all kinds of scientific and quasi-scientific controversies. But it turns out we don't really have to worry about any of this because the world's going to end in 2012. Another update you will recall.
So I introduced you guys to the quadro tracker. It's a, um, like a water dowsing device. It's just a hollow piece of plastic with an antenna that swivels around, and you walk around, and it points to things. Like, if you're looking for marijuana in students' lockers, it'll, you know, like point right to somebody. Oh, sorry. This particular one that was given to me finds golf balls, especially if you're at a golf course and you check under enough bushes.
Well, under the category of "what's the harm of silly stuff like this," this device, the AD 651, was sold to the Iraqi government for $440,000 a piece. It's just like this one: completely worthless, in which it allegedly worked by electrostatic magnetic ion attraction, which translates to pseudoscientific baloney, would be the nice word. You string together a bunch of words that sound good, but it does absolutely nothing. In this case, allowing, at trespass points, allowing people to go through because your little tracker device said they were okay actually cost lives.
So there is a danger to pseudoscience and believing in this sort of thing. So what I want to talk about today is belief. I want to believe, and you do too. In fact, I think my thesis here is that belief is the natural state of things; it is the default option. We just believe. We believe all sorts of things. Belief is natural; disbelief, skepticism, science is not natural. It's more difficult; it's uncomfortable to not believe things.
So, like Fox Mulder on X-Files, who wants to believe in UFOs—well, we all do. And the reason for that is because we are, uh, we have a belief engine in our brains, essentially. We are pattern-seeking primates. We connect the dots: A is connected to B, B is connected to C, and sometimes A really is connected to B, and that's called association learning. We find patterns, we make those connections, whether it's Pavlov's dog here, associating the sound of the bell with the food, and then he salivates to the sound of the bell, or whether it's a Scarab rat, in which he's having an association between his behavior and a reward for it, and therefore he repeats the behavior.
In fact, what Skinner discovered is that if you put a pigeon in a box like this, and he has to press one of these two keys and he tries to figure out what the pattern is, and you give them a little reward in the hopper box there, if you just randomly assign rewards such that there is no pattern, they will figure out any kind of pattern. Whatever they were doing just before they got the reward, they repeat that particular pattern. Sometimes it was even spinning around twice counterclockwise, once clockwise, and peck the key twice, and that's called superstition.
And that, I'm afraid, we will always have with us. I call this process patternicity; that is the tendency to find meaningful patterns in both meaningful and meaningless noise. When we do this process, we make two types of errors. A type one error, or false positive, is believing a pattern is real when it's not. Our second type of error, a false negative—a type two error—is not believing a pattern is real when it is.
So let's do a thought experiment. You are a hominid 3 million years ago, walking on the plains of Africa. Your name is Lucy, okay? And you hear a rustle in the grass. Is it a dangerous predator, or is it just the wind? Your next decision could be the most important one of your life. Well, if you think that the rustle in the grass is a dangerous predator and it turns out it's just the wind, you've made an error in cognition; you made a type one error, false positive. But no harm, you just move away; you're more cautious, you're more vigilant.
On the other hand, if you believe that the rustle in the grass is just the wind, and it turns out it's a dangerous predator, you're lunch. You've just won a Darwin Award; you've been taken out of the gene pool. Now, the problem here is that patternicity will occur whenever the cost of making a type one error is less than the cost of making a type two error.
This is the only equation in the talk, by the way. We have a pattern detection problem, that is assessing the difference between a type one and a type two error is highly problematic, especially in split-second life-and-death situations. So the default position is just to believe all patterns are real. All rustles in the grass are dangerous predators and not just the wind.
I think that we evolved. There was natural selection for the propensity for our belief engines, our pattern-seeking brain processes, to always find meaningful patterns and infuse them with these sort of predatory or intentional agencies. That'll come back to. So for example, what do you see here? It's a horse head. That's right; it looks like a horse. Must be a horse. That's a pattern.
And is it really a horse, or is it more like a frog? See, our pattern detection device, which appears to be located in the anterior cingulate cortex, is our little sort of detection device there. It can be easily fooled, and this is the problem. For example, what do you see here? Yes, of course, it's a cow. Once I prime the brain—it's called cognitive priming. Once I prime the brain to see it, it pops back out again even without the pattern that I've imposed on it.
And what do you see here? Some people see a Dalmatian dog. Yes, there it is, and there's the prime. So when I go back without the prime, your brain already has the model, so you can see it again. What do you see here? Planet Saturn? Yes, that's good. How about here? Just shout out anything you see. That's a good audience, Chris, because there's nothing in this. Well, allegedly there's nothing. This is an experiment done by Jennifer Witson at, um, at UT Austin on corporate environments and whether uncertain feelings of uncertainty and out-of-control people see illusory patterns.
That is, almost everybody sees the planet Saturn. People that are put in a condition of feeling out of control are more likely to see something in this, which is allegedly patternless. In other words, the propensity to feel these, or find these patterns, goes up when there's a lack of control. For example, baseball players are notoriously superstitious when they're batting, but not so much when they're fielding because fielders are successful 90 to 95% of the time. The best batters fail seven out of ten times.
So their superstitions, their patternicity, are all associated with feelings of lack of control and so forth. What do you see in this particular one here in this field? Anybody see an object there? There actually is something here, but it's degraded. While you're thinking about that, this was an experiment done by Susan Blackmore, a psychologist in England, who showed subjects this degraded image and then ran a correlation between their scores on an ESP test—how much do they believe in the paranormal, supernatural, angels, and so forth—and those who scored high on the ESP scale tended to see not only see more patterns in the degraded images but incorrect patterns.
Here is what, uh, you show subjects the fish. It's degraded 20%, 50%, and then the one I showed you: 70%. A similar experiment was done by another British psychologist named Peter Brer, who found significantly more patterns, meaningful patterns, are perceived on the right hemisphere via the left visual field than the left hemisphere. So if you present subjects the images such that it's going to end up on the right hemisphere instead of the left, then they're more likely to see patterns than if you put it on the left hemisphere.
Our right hemisphere appears to be where a lot of this patternicity occurs. So what we're trying to do is bore into the brain to see where all this happens. Breer and his colleague Christine Moore gave subjects L-DOPA. L-DOPA is a drug, as you know, given for treating Parkinson's disease, which is related to a decrease in dopamine. L-DOPA increases dopamine. An increase in dopamine caused subjects to see more patterns than those that did not receive the dopamine.
So dopamine appears to be the drug associated with patternicity. In fact, neuroleptic drugs that are used to eliminate psychotic behavior—things like paranoia, delusions, and hallucinations—these are patternicity; they're incorrect patterns. They're false positives or type one errors, and if you give them drugs that are dopamine antagonists, they go away; that is, you decrease the amount of dopamine, and their tendency to see patterns like that decreases. On the other hand, amphetamines like cocaine are dopamine agonists; they increase the amount of dopamine, so you're more likely to feel in a euphoric state, creatively find more patterns.
In fact, I saw Robin Williams recently talk about how he was thought he was much funnier when he was doing cocaine, when he had that issue, than now. So perhaps more dopamine is related to more creativity. Dopamine, I think, changes our signal-to-noise ratio; that is, how accurate we are in finding patterns. If it's too low, you're more likely to make too many type two errors; you miss the real patterns. You don't want to be too skeptical; if you're too skeptical, you miss the really interesting good ideas.
Just right, you're creative, and yet you don't fall for too much baloney. Too high, and maybe you see patterns everywhere. Every time somebody looks at you, you think people are staring at you; you think people are talking about you. And if you go too far on that, that's just simply labeled as madness. It's a distinction perhaps we might make between two neurobiologists: Richard Feynman and John Nash. One sees maybe just the right number of patterns to win a Nobel Prize; the other one also, but maybe too many patterns, and we then call that schizophrenia.
So the signal-to-noise ratio then presents us with a pattern detection problem. And of course, you all know exactly what this is, right? And what pattern do you see here? Again, I'm putting your anterior cingulate cortex to the test here, causing you conflicting pattern detections. You know of course this is Via Uno shoes; these are sandals, pretty sexy feet, I must say. Maybe a little photoshopped. And of course, the ambiguous figures that seem to flip-flop back and forth.
Turns out what you're thinking about a lot influences what you tend to see. You see the lamp here, I know, because I have lights on here. Of course, thanks to the environmentalist movement, we're all sensitive to the plight of marine mammals. So what you see in this particular ambiguous figure is, of course, the dolphins, right? You see a dolphin here, and there's a dolphin, and there's a dolphin. That's a dolphin tail there.
[Applause] Guys, um, if we can give you conflicting data, again your ACC is going to be going into hyperdrive. If you look down here, it's fine; if you look up here, then you get conflicting data, and then we have to flip the image for you to see that it's a set-up: the impossible crate illusion. It's easy to fool the brain in 2D, so you say, "Oh, come on, anybody can do that in a Psych 101 text with an illusion like that."
Well, here's the late great Jerry Andrew's impossible crate illusion in 3D, in which Jerry is standing inside the impossible crate, and he was kind enough to post this and give us the reveal. Of course, camera angle is everything; the photographer is over there, and this board appears to overlap with this one and this one with that one, and so on. But even when I take it away, the illusion is so powerful because of how our brains are wired to find those certain kinds of patterns.
This is a fairly new one that throws us off because of the conflicting patterns of comparing this angle with that angle. In fact, it's the exact same picture side by side. So what you're doing is comparing that angle instead of this one, but with that one, and so your brain is fooled yet again. Your pattern detection devices are fooled. Faces are easy to see because we have an additional evolved facial recognition software in our temporal lobes.
Here are some faces on the side of a rock. I'm actually not even sure if this might be Photoshopped, but anyway, the point is still made. Which one of these looks odd to you? In a quick reaction, which one looks odd? The one on the left? Okay, I'll rotate it, so it'll be the one on the right, and you are correct, a fairly famous illusion that was first done with Margaret Thatcher. Now they trade up to politicians every time.
Well, why is this happening? Well, we know exactly where it happens—in the temporal lobe, right across sort of above your ear there, in a little structure called the fusiform gyrus. There are two types of cells that do this that record facial features, either globally or specifically. These large rapid-firing cells first look at the general face, so you recognize Obama immediately.
Then you notice something quite a little bit odd about the eyes and the mouth, especially when they're upside down; you're engaging that general facial recognition software there. Now I said, back in our little thought experiment, you're a hominid walking on the plains of Africa. Is it just the wind or a dangerous predator? What's the difference between those? Well, a wind is an inanimate, a dangerous predator is an intentional agent.
I call this process agenticity; that is, the tendency to infuse patterns with meaning, intention, and agency—often invisible beings—from the top down. This is an idea that we got from a fellow TEDster here, Dan Dennett, talking about taking the intentional stance. So it's a type of that expanded to explain, I think, a lot of different things: souls, spirits, ghosts, gods, demons, angels, aliens, intelligent designers, government conspiracists, and all manner of invisible agents with power and intention.
They are believed to haunt our world and control our lives. I think it's the basis of animism and polytheism and monotheism. It's the belief that aliens are somehow more advanced than us, more moral than us in the Aesop fables, and that they're coming here to save us and rescue us from on high. The intelligent designer is always portrayed as this super intelligent moral being that comes down to design life.
Even the idea that government can rescue us—that's no longer the wave of the future, but that is, I think, a type of agenticity: the projecting somebody up there, big and powerful, will come rescue us. And this is also, I think, the basis of conspiracy theories. There's somebody hiding behind there pulling the strings, whether it's the Illuminati or the Bilderbergers. But this is a pattern detection problem, isn't it? Some patterns are real, and some are not.
Was JFK assassinated by a conspiracy or by a lone assassin? Well, if you go there, there's people there on any given day, like when I went there, showing me where the different shooters were. My favorite one was he was in the manhole and he popped out at the last second, took that shot. But of course, Lincoln was assassinated by a conspiracy, so we can't just uniformly dismiss all patterns like that because let's face it, some patterns are real; some conspiracies really are true.
Explains a lot, maybe, and 911 as a conspiracy theory? It is a conspiracy. We did a whole issue on it—19 members of Al-Qaeda plotting to fly planes into buildings constitutes a conspiracy. But that's not what the 911 truthers think. They think it was an inside job by the Bush Administration. Well, that's a whole another lecture, but you know how we know that 911 was not orchestrated by the Bush Administration? Because it worked.
We are natural-born dualists. Our agenticity process comes from the fact that we can enjoy movies like these because we can imagine an essence continuing on. We know that if you stimulate the temporal lobe, you can produce a feeling of out-of-body experiences, near-death experiences, which you can do by just touching an electrode to the temporal lobe there. Or you can do it through loss of consciousness by accelerating in a centrifuge; you get a hypoxia or lower oxygen, and the brain then senses that there's an out-of-body experience.
You can use, which I did—went up and did Michael Persinger's God helmet that bombards your temporal lobes with electromagnetic waves, and you get a sense of out-of-body experience. So I'm going to end here with a short video clip that sort of brings all this together. It's just a minute and a half that ties together all this into the power of expectation and the power of belief. Go ahead and roll it.
This is the venue they chose for their fake auditions for an advert for lip balm. We're hoping that we can use part of this in a national commercial, right? And this is a test on some lip balms that we have over here, and these are our models who are going to help us—Roger and Matt. Okay, would you have any problem kissing our models to test you? No? You think that was fine? That would be fine?
Okay, so this is a blind test. I'm going to ask you to go ahead and put a blindfold on. Okay, now, can you see anything? No. Hold up. So you can't even see down? Okay, it's completely blind now, right? Okay. Now what I'm going to be looking for in this test is, um, makes the lips okay, the texture, right? Maybe if you can discern any flavor or not. Have you ever done a kissing test before? You excited?
Take one step in. Okay, now I'm going to ask you to pucker up. Pucker up big and lean in just a little bit. Okay? Okay, all right. [Music] Okay, and Jennifer, how did that feel? [Music] Good. Oh my God, thank you very much. Thank you. Thanks. Thanks.