Txtng is killing language. JK!!! - John McWhorter
[Music] [Music] We always hear that texting is a scourge. The idea is that texting spells the decline and fall of any kind of serious literacy or at least writing ability among young people in the United States and now the whole world. Today, the fact of the matter is that it just isn't true.
It's easy to think that it is true, but in order to see it in another way, in order to see that actually texting is a miraculous thing—not just energetic, but a miraculous thing, a kind of emergent complexity that we're seeing happening right now—we have to pull the camera back for a bit and look at what language really is.
In which case, one thing that we see is that texting is not writing at all. What do I mean by that? Basically, if we think about language, language has existed for perhaps a hundred and fifty thousand years—at least eighty thousand years. What it arose as is speech; people talked. That's what we're probably genetically specified for. That's how we use language.
Most writing is something that came along much later, and as we saw in the last talk, there's a little bit of controversy as to exactly when that happened. But according to traditional estimates, if humanity had existed for 24 hours, then writing only came along at about 11:07 p.m. That's how much of a laterally thing writing is.
So first there’s speech, and then writing comes along as a kind of artifice. Now, don't get me wrong; writing has certain advantages. When you write, because it's a conscious process, because you can look backwards, you can do things with language that are much less likely if you're just talking. For example, imagine a passage from Edward Gibbons, The Decline and Fall of the Roman Empire: "The whole engagement lasted above 12 hours till the gradual retreat of the Persians was changed into a disorderly flight of which the shameful examples given by the principal leaders in the serena's himself."
That's—but let's face it, nobody talks that way, or at least they shouldn't if they're interested in reproducing that. It's not the way any human being speaks casually. Casual speech is something quite different. Linguists have actually shown that when we're speaking casually in an unmonitored way, we tend to speak in word packets of maybe seven to ten words. You'll notice this if you ever have occasion to record yourself or a group of people talking. That's what speech is like; speech is much looser, it's much more telegraphic, it's much less reflective—very different from writing.
So we naturally tend to think, because we see language written so often, that that's what language is. But actually, what language is, is speech. They're two things. Now, of course, as history has gone by, it's been natural for there to be a certain amount of bleed between speech and writing. So, for example, in a distant era now, it was common when one gave a speech to basically talk like writing.
I mean the kind of speech that you see someone giving in an old movie where they clear their throat and say, "Ladies and gentlemen," and then they speak in a certain way which has nothing to do with casual speech. It's formal; it uses long sentences like this one. It's basically talking like you write.
And so, for example, we're thinking so much these days about Lincoln because of the movie. The Gettysburg Address was not the main meal of that event. For two hours before that, Edward Everett spoke on a topic that frankly cannot engage us today, and barely did then. The point of it was to listen to him speaking like writing. Ordinary people stood and listened to that for two hours. It was perfectly natural; that's what people did then.
Speaking like writing—well, if you can speak like writing, then logically it follows that you might want to also sometimes write like you speak. The problem was just that in the material mechanical sense, that was harder back in the day for the simple reason that materials don't lend themselves to it. It's almost impossible to do that with your hand except in shorthand, and then communication is limited.
On a manual typewriter, it was very difficult, and even when we had electric typewriters or then computer keyboards, the fact is that even if you can type easily enough to keep up with the pace of speech, more or less, you have to have somebody who can receive your message quickly. Once you have things in your pocket that can receive that message, then you have the conditions that allow that we can write like we speak, and that's where texting comes in.
Texting is very loose in its structure; no one thinks about capital letters or punctuation when one texts. But then again, do you think about those things when you talk? No! And so therefore, why would you when you were texting? What texting is, despite the fact that it involves the brute mechanics of something that we call writing, is fingered speech. That's what texting is.
Now, we can write the way we talk, and it's a very interesting thing. But nevertheless, it's easy to think that still it represents some sort of decline. We see this general bagginess of the structure, the lack of concern with rules in the way that we're used to learning on the blackboard, and so we think that something has gone wrong. It's a very natural sense, but the fact of the matter is that what is going on is a kind of emergent complexity.
That's what we're seeing in this fingered speech. In order to understand it, what we want to see is the way in this new kind of language there is new structure coming up. And so, for example, there is in texting a convention which is "lol." Now, "lol," we generally think of as meaning "laughing out loud," and of course theoretically it does. If you look at older texts, people used it to actually indicate laughing out loud.
But if you text now, or if you are someone who's aware of the substrate of texting, you'll notice that "lol" does not mean laughing out loud anymore. It's evolved into something that is much subtler. This is an actual text that was done by a non-male person of about 20 years old not too long ago: "I love the font you're using bTW. Julie Lal thanks. Gmail is being slow right now."
Have you thought about it? That's not funny; no one's laughing! And yet there it is. So you assume there's been some kind of hiccup. Then Susan says "lawl." I know again more guffawing than we're used to when you're talking about these inconveniences. So Julie says, "I just sent you an email." Susan: "lul, I see it."
Very funny, people—if that's what "lol" means. This Julie says, "So what's up?" Susan: "Lau, I have to write a 10-page paper." She's not amused. Let's think about it: "lul" is being used in a very particular way. It's a marker of empathy; it's a marker of accommodation. We linguists call things like that pragmatic particles.
Any spoken language that's used by real people has them. If you happen to speak Japanese, think about that little word "ne" that you use at the end of a lot of sentences. If you listen to the way Black youth today speak, think about the use of the word "yo." Hold dissertations could be written about it, and probably are being written about it. A pragmatic particle—that's what "lal" has gradually become. It's a way of using the language between actual people.
Another example is "slash." Now, we can use "slash" in the way that we're used to along the lines of, "we're going to have a party slash networking session." That's kind of like what we're at. "Slash" is used in a very different way in texting among young people today. It's used to change the scene.
So, for example, this Sally person says, "So I need to find people to chill with," and Jake says, "Haha, you'd write a dissertation about 'haha'—but we don't have time for that. Haha." So you're going by yourself? Why Sally for this summer program at NYU? Jake: "Haha / I'm watching this video with Suns players trying to shoot with one eye."
The slash is interesting; I don't really even know what Jake is talking about after that. But you notice that he's changing the topic now. That seems kind of mundane, but think about how in real life if we're having a conversation we want to change the topic. There are ways of doing it gracefully; you don't just zip right into it.
You'll pat your thighs and look wistfully off into the distance, or you'll say something like, "Makes you think," when it really didn't. But what you're really trying to do is change the topic. You can't do that while you're texting, and so ways are developing of doing it within this medium. All spoken languages have what a linguist calls a new information marker or two or three.
Texting has developed one from this "slash." So we have a whole battery of new constructions that are developing, and yet it's easy to think, "Well, something is still wrong; there's a lack of structure of some sort." It's not as sophisticated as the language of the Wall Street Journal. Well, the fact of the matter is, look at this person in 1956, and this is when texting doesn’t exist.
I Love Lucy is still on the air. Many do not know the alphabet or multiplication table, cannot write formally. We've heard that sort of thing before—not just in 1956; 1917, Connecticut schoolteacher. 1917—this is the time when we all assumed that everything somehow, in terms of writing, was perfect because the people on Downton Abbey were articulate or something like that. So from every college in the country goes up the cry: "Our freshmen can’t spell, can’t punctuate," and so on.
You can go even further back than this. It's the president of Harvard in 1871; there's no electricity. People have three names. Bad spelling, incorrectness, as well as in elegance of expression in writing. And he's talking about people who are otherwise well prepared for college studies. You can go even further back: 1841, some long-lost Superintendent of Schools is upset because of what he has for a long time noted with regret—the almost entire neglect of the original... blah blah blah blah, or you can go all the way back to 63 AD, and there's this poor man who doesn't like the way people are speaking Latin.
As it happens, he was writing about what had become French. And there are always people worrying about these things, and the planet somehow seems to keep spinning.
So the way I'm thinking of texting these days is that what we're seeing is a whole new way of writing that young people are developing, which they're using alongside their ordinary writing skills. And that means that they're able to do two things: increasing evidence is that being bilingual is cognitively beneficial.
That's also true of being bidialectal; that's certainly true of being bidialectal in terms of your writing. And so texting actually is evidence of a balancing act that young people are using today—not consciously, of course—but it's an expansion of their linguistic repertoire.
It's very simple: if somebody from 1973 looked at what was on a dormitory message board in 1993, the slang would have changed a little bit since the era of Love Story, but they would understand what was on that message board. Take that person from 1993—not that long ago; this is, you know, Bill & Ted's Excellent Adventure. Those people take those people and they read a very typical text written by a 20-year-old today. Often, they would have no idea what half of it meant because a whole new language has developed among our young people doing something as mundane as what it looks like to us when they're batting around on their little devices.
So in closing, if I could go into the future, if I could go into 2033, the first thing I would ask is whether David Simon had done a sequel to The Wire. I would want to know, and I really would ask that. And then I’d want to know actually what was going on on Downton Abbey. That'd be the second thing. And then the third thing would be, "Please show me a sheaf of texts written by 16-year-old girls," because I would want to know where this language had developed since our times.
And ideally, I would then send them back to you and me now, so we could examine this linguistic miracle happening right under our noses. Thank you very much!