yego.me
💡 Stop wasting time. Read Youtube instead of watch. Download Chrome Extension

How Hope Affects Relationships: Expectations, Obstacles, and Sharing Optimism | Eshkol Rafaeli


6m read
·Nov 3, 2024

My name is Eshkol Rafaeli. I’m a psychologist and a relationship researcher from Bar-Ilan University in Israel, and I’ve been involved with the Hope and Optimism Initiative for the past couple of years as one of the grantees on the social science side. This initiative funded a research project that we are nearing the completion of now.

It’s a study looking at couples in the transition to parenthood, couples who are expecting their first child, whom we follow for a period of time, starting in the third trimester of pregnancy all the way through, at this point, six months postpartum. We’re actually hoping to continue this work and be able to follow them through one year and then two years after the child is born.

We do this work both because we’re really interested in this population of couples in this juncture of time. It is so full of expectations but also fears and dreads. So we’re interested in the population, and we’re also interested in this quality of hope and trying to see—through the use of this population—to see something about the nature of hope.

To try to understand something about what hope is at the level of not just one individual but, in this case, a couple or family, an emerging family. We have three premises that guide our work. Two of them are already underway, and the third one is more of a future direction that we’re hoping to pursue.

The two that are underway are looking at hope as more than just a stable quality, an individual characteristic that differentiates one person from another or maybe one couple from another, instead recognizing that hope has this fluctuating component to it—that it could change over time. There are ways in which the stability or instability in it beyond just a set level could be really interesting and important. So this dynamic quality of hope is one thing we’re looking at.

And we do that by having these partners, these couples, tell us about their hope, expectation, and optimism repeatedly. We do this both in the long range of things, so following them from prepartum through postpartum over the course of these basically nine months, from the third trimester through six months after birth. But also, for a period of time around three months after they give birth, we have them complete daily diaries for a period of three weeks where we look at fluctuations on a daily basis.

We ask them to report their own feelings and to impute feelings about their partner so that we’ll be able to see how these things change from moment to moment. So that’s one premise or one idea that we have in our work. A second idea that I alluded to earlier is the idea that hope is more than just an individual characteristic, that there’s something about it that could be interesting to explore or to understand at the level of a system, a dyad in this case—a pair of people.

We’re curious whether hope is contagious. For that, you need these two people’s reports of their own hopes to see whether one person’s hope affects the other over time. We were wondering whether it’s compensatory, whether the presence of hope for one could carry them through difficult periods like the sleepless days that come after birth or the prepartum period where there’s often a lot of anxiety about what this will be like, what parenting will become like.

Is it enough for one person to have hope for both partners, or do we really need it to be high for both? Those are two premises that we have the data for, at least in part. We’re still waiting for the last of those couples in our sample to complete the three-month and six-month postpartum, but thankfully all the people that we recruited in this study have, by now, given birth to healthy babies, which was a relief.

And we’ve been starting to look at the data mostly from the prepartum phase at this point—so the data before the child was born. And we saw some interesting things already. One of them is that it seemed that it’s the expectant mother's hope, much more so than the expectant father's, that seems to have effects both for these expectant mothers themselves—their sense of relationship satisfaction, their sense of well-being in the world was tied to their own hope—but so were their partners'.

So the women’s hope seems to have both a direct effect on their own feelings, well-being, satisfaction, and on their partners. And we didn’t find the counterpart effect from the men’s hope. In other words, there’s something about this period—maybe—we’ll have to see whether it’s just this period—that gives extra weight to hope on these expectant mothers more than the expectant fathers' side.

One of the things we want to explore is whether this will shift, maybe even flip over or at least balance itself out postpartum. We think it might. We think that there’s something about the kind of experience that expectant mothers have that, just by the sheer fact that they are the ones carrying the baby, makes them much more connected to thinking about what parenthood will be like, thinking about the baby.

And the fathers, in a sense, may not be quite getting it until after childbirth happens, after they become parents. So we’re expecting that something will change with this postpartum, and maybe that the greater exhaustion that new mothers seem to have may wash out this effect of hope. Maybe there won’t be much hope—we do see some drops in hope postpartum from what they’ve peaked at, but really it’s too early to tell whether it’s really a serious drop or just a minor drop in hope levels.

Okay, so these two premises that we’ve been exploring in data are that hope fluctuates, that it’s dynamic, and that hope is a quality of the couple—that it’s dyadic. We have a third premise, but that’s one that we’re going to be exploring in our next project. We’re really interested to see whether hope is malleable. Whether we can change it.

And to do that, we’re planning a study that applies some research on mental imagery; hope has a strong imagery component as many of the other speakers at this festival have talked about. There’s a daydreaming component. There’s an imaginal component to hope, and we are emphasizing this imaginal component in this next piece of our work.

We want to see whether we can train people, in this case couples, to imagine their desired outcomes—these things that they hope for—in a more or less effective way. We’re going to be doing this as a randomized study where some of the couples receive this seemingly logical intervention of, like, "Let’s imagine the future and let’s think about these positive outcomes that will happen," which on the face of it sounds like it would be a way to increase hope.

But in reality it’s going to be—or we think is going to be—less effective than a different kind of intervention where we have them imagine these desired outcomes, but we also have them imagine the obstacles and the way to get around these obstacles to get to these outcomes. Work from motivational psychologists and health psychologists has shown that this sort of process—it’s often called mental contrasting—where you think about what you want, you really imagine it, but you also think about what’s going to block your progress towards that desired goal leads to more effective hope.

Leads to more effective accomplishment of these things that people want for themselves. We want to see whether this happens here. We want to see whether doing this in the context of a relationship, where we’ll have one of, probably one of my graduate students, Ph.D. students in psychology, delivering this, but in the presence of the partner as well.

So we’re going to have both partners in the room, and we’ll switch and do a similar kind of intervention with both of them in mind. Whether this kind of work can actually increase hope in a sustainable way and lead them to accomplish or fulfill the expectations that they have for what it will be like to be parents and for what it will be like to be a couple after the child has arrived in a more effective way.

More Articles

View All
The Saltwater Croc Threat | Primal Survivor
I’m traveling along the Araund River in Papua New Guinea, and I’m now over halfway to my destination—a village that a generation ago practiced one of the darkest customs of all: cannibalism. A place where young men would be sent out to bring back the head…
Lecture: Biblical Series IV: Adam and Eve: Self-Consciousness, Evil, and Death
Hello everyone, so hopefully, we’re going to get past Genesis 1 today. That’s the theory. So I finished my new book yesterday. Yes, that’s taken about three years of writing. It’s quite a long time to write something, so yeah, it’s done except for the mop…
How to Bring Mastery Learning to Your Class... And Get Results Like Tim's!
Hi everyone! This is Jeremy Schiefling with Khan Academy. I want to thank you for joining us this afternoon or this evening depending on where you’re calling in from, and you are in for a very special treat. So as you probably know, we’ve been doing webi…
The Real Reason Jonah was Swallowed by the Whale
[Applause] So I’m going to tell you another story. And this time, why this story? Because it brings together themes of the highest possible aim, responsibility and meaning, and cultural renewal. And it’s an amazing story. It’s the story of Jonah, and I li…
How long should your naps be? - Sara C. Mednick
Your eyes get heavy and gradually… close… But wait! It’s only lunchtime and you still have so much to do. Would taking a nap help? Or would it derail your day? Our sleep, both at night and in naps, is made up of approximately 90 minute sleep cycles with …
Indoor air pollutants| Atmospheric pollution| AP Environmental science| Khan Academy
Let’s talk about indoor air pollution. I remember when I first heard about indoor air pollution in my AP Environmental Science class, I was a little confused. When I used to think of pollution, I would think of images like this or this. But pollution is o…