yego.me
💡 Stop wasting time. Read Youtube instead of watch. Download Chrome Extension

Are all of your memories real? - Daniel L. Schacter


3m read
·Nov 8, 2024

In a study in the 1990s, participants recalled getting lost in a shopping mall as children. Some shared these memories in vivid detail— one even remembered that the old man who rescued him was wearing a flannel shirt. But none of these people had actually gotten lost in a mall. They produced these false memories when the psychologists conducting the study told them they’d gotten lost, and although they might not remember the incident, their parents had confirmed it. And it wasn’t just one or two people who thought they remembered getting lost— a quarter of the participants did.

These findings may sound unbelievable, but they actually reflect a very common experience. Our memories are sometimes unreliable. And though we still don’t know precisely what causes this fallibility on a neurological level, research has highlighted some of the most common ways our memories diverge from what actually happened. The mall study highlights how we can incorporate information from outside sources, like other people or the news, into our personal recollections without realizing it. This kind of suggestibility is just one influence on our memories.

Take another study, in which researchers briefly showed a random collection of photographs to a group of participants, including images of a university campus none of them had ever visited. When shown the images three weeks later, a majority of participants said that they had probably or definitely visited the campus in the past. The participants misattributed information from one context— an image they’d seen— onto another— a memory of something they believed they actually experienced.

In another experiment, people were shown an image of a magnifying glass, and then told to imagine a lollipop. They frequently recalled that they saw the magnifying glass and the lollipop. They struggled to link the objects to the correct context— whether they actually saw them or simply imagined them. Another study, where a psychologist questioned over 2,000 people on their views about the legalization of marijuana, highlights yet another kind of influence on memory.

Participants answered questions in 1973 and 1982. Those who said they had supported marijuana legalization in 1973, but reported they were against it in 1982, were more likely to recall that they were actually against legalization in 1973— bringing their old views in line with their current ones. Our current opinions, feelings, and experiences can bias our memories of how we felt in the past.

In another study, researchers gave two groups of participants background information on a historical war and asked them to rate the likelihood that each side would win. They gave each group the same information, except that they only told one group who had actually won the war— the other group didn’t know the real-world outcome. In theory, both groups' answers should be similar because the likelihood of each side winning isn’t affected by who actually won— if there’s a 20% chance of thunderstorms, and a thunderstorm happens, the chance of thunderstorms doesn’t retroactively go up to 100%.

Still, the group that knew how the war ended rated the winning side as more likely to win than the group who did not. All of these fallibilities of memory can have real-world impacts. If police interrogations use leading questions with eyewitnesses or suspects, suggestibility could result in incorrect identifications or unreliable confessions. Even in the absence of leading questions, misattribution can lead to inaccurate eyewitness testimony.

In a courtroom, if a judge rules a piece of evidence inadmissible and tells jurors to disregard it, they may not be able to do so. In a medical setting, if a patient seeks a second opinion and the second physician is aware of the first one’s diagnosis, that knowledge may bias their conclusion. Our memories are not ironclad representations of reality, but subjective perceptions. And there’s not necessarily anything wrong with that— the problems arise when we treat memory as fact, rather than accepting this fundamental truth about the nature of our recollections.

More Articles

View All
2019 Berkshire Hathaway Annual Meeting (Full Version)
Thank you, good morning and welcome to Berkshire Hathaway. For those of you who have come from out of state, welcome to Omaha. The city is delighted to have you here for this event. For those of you who came from outside of the country, welcome to the Un…
Game theory worked example from A P Microeconomics
What we have here is a free response question that you might see on an AP Microeconomics type exam that deals with game theory. It tells us Bread Basket and Quick Lunch are the only two sandwich shops serving a small town, so we’re in an oligopoly situati…
15 Things Rich People Advise But Never Do
Everyone looks for advice from the rich, but advice is not universally applicable, and even they don’t follow it, and for good reason. Here are 15 things rich people advise but never actually do. Welcome to Alux. Number one: go to school. Going to school…
Dave Bautista Makes a Log Ladder | Running Wild With Bear Grylls
[music playing] So the terrain here is definitely getting steeper and more committing. But you know, so much of survival is about just trying to be resourceful. I just wonder if maybe we maybe use that old trunk. Use that, get that down, and then we can d…
2015 AP Physics 1 free response 1a
Two blocks are connected by a string of negligible mass that passes over a massless pulley that turns with negligible friction. It is shown in the figure above. We see that the mass M2 of block 2 is greater than the mass M1 of block 1. The blocks are rele…
What’s private or incognito mode?
So Kelly, one thing that I see on a lot of browsers, they might have like a private mode, or an incognito mode. What do those things generally do, and how private, or incognito are they, and what should we still think about, even if we are using those mod…