yego.me
💡 Stop wasting time. Read Youtube instead of watch. Download Chrome Extension

Your brain on improv - Charles Limb


11m read
·Nov 8, 2024

[Music] [Music] [Applause] [Music] So I am a surgeon who studies creativity, and I have never had a patient tell me, “I really want you to be creative during surgery.” And so I guess there's a little bit of irony to it. I will say, though, that after having done surgery a lot, it's somewhat similar to playing a musical instrument. For me, this sort of deep and enduring fascination with sound is what led me to both be a surgeon and also to study the science of sound, particularly music.

And so I'm going to try to talk to you over the next few minutes about my career in terms of how I'm able to actually try to study music and really try to grapple with these questions of how the brain is able to be creative. I've done most of this work at Johns Hopkins University, but also at the National Institutes of Health where I was previously. I'm going to go over some science experiments and try to cover three musical experiments.

I'm going to start off by playing a video for you. This video is a video of Keith Jarrett, who's a well-known jazz improviser and probably the most well-known iconic example of someone who takes improvisation to a really higher level. He'll improvise entire concerts off the top of his head, and he'll never play it exactly the same way again. So as a form of intense creativity, I think this is a great example. And so why don't we go ahead and click the video?

[Music] Clearly, a remarkable, awesome thing that happens there. I've always, just as a listener, as just a fan, I listen to that and I'm just astounded. I think, how can this possibly be? How can the brain generate that much information, that much music spontaneously? And so I set out with this concept scientifically that artistic creativity—it's magical, but it's not magic. I mean that it's a product of the brain. There are not too many brain-dead people creating art.

And so with this notion that artistic creativity is in fact a neurologic product, I took this thesis that we could study it just like we study any other complex neurologic process. I think there are some sub-questions there that I put forth: Is it truly possible to study creativity scientifically? And I think that's a good question. I'll tell you that most scientific studies of music are very dense, and when you actually go through them, it’s very hard to recognize the music in it. In fact, they seem to be very unmusical entirely and to miss the whole point of the music.

And so it brings up the second question: Why should scientists study creativity? Maybe we're not the right people to do it. Well, it may be, but I will say that from a scientific perspective, we talked a lot about innovation today. The science of innovation—how much we understand about how the brain is able to innovate is in its infancy, and truly we know very little about how we are able to be creative. So I think that we're going to see over the next 10, 20, 30 years a real science of creativity that's burgeoning and is going to flourish because we now have new methods that can enable us to take this process of something like this complex jazz improvisation and study it rigorously.

And so it gets down to the brain. All of us have this remarkable brain, which is poorly understood, to say the least. I think that neuroscientists have much more questions than answers, and I myself am not going to give you many answers today; just ask a lot of questions. Fundamentally, that's what I do in my lab: ask questions about what is this brain doing to enable us to do this?

This is the main method that I use. This is called functional MRI. If you’ve been in an MRI scanner, it’s very much the same, but this one is outfitted in a special way to not just take pictures of your brain, but to also take pictures of active areas of the brain. Now, the way that's done is by the following: There’s something called BOLD imaging, which is blood oxygen level dependent imaging.

Now, when you're in an fMRI scanner, you're in a big magnet that's aligning your molecules in certain areas. When an area of the brain is active—meaning a neural area is active—it gets blood flow shunted to that area. That blood flow causes an increase in local blood to that area with a deoxyhemoglobin change in concentration. Deoxyhemoglobin can be detected by MRI, whereas oxyhemoglobin can't. So through this sort of method of inference, and we're measuring blood flow, not neural activity, we say that an area of the brain that's getting more blood was active during a particular task, and that's sort of the crux of how fMRI works. It’s been used since the '90s to study really complex processes.

Now I'm going to review a study that I did, which was really jazz in an fMRI scanner. This was done with a colleague of mine, Alan Brown, at the NIH. This is a short video of how we did this project. So this is a plastic MIDI piano keyboard that we used for the jazz experiments, and it's a 35-key keyboard that is designed to fit both inside the scanner, be magnetically safe, have minimal interference that would contribute to any artifact, and have this cushion so that it can rest on the player's legs while they're lying down in the scanner, playing on their back.

And it works like this: This doesn't actually produce any sound. It sends out what's called a MIDI signal, or musical instrument digital interface, through these wires into the box and the computer, which then triggers high-quality piano samples like this.

[Music] Okay, so it works. And so through this piano keyboard, we now have a means to actually take a musical process and study it. So what do you do now that you have this cool piano keyboard? You can't just sort of, you know, it’s great; we got this keyboard; we actually have to come up with a scientific experiment.

And so the experiment really rests on the following: What happens in the brain during something that's memorized and overlearned, and what happens in the brain during something that is spontaneously generated or improvised, in a way that's matched motorically and in terms of lower-level sensory motor features?

I have here what we call the paradigms. There’s a scale paradigm, which is just playing a scale up and down, memorized. And then there’s improvising on a scale, quarter notes metronome, right hand—scientifically very safe, but musically really boring. And then there’s the bottom one, which is called the jazz paradigm.

So what we did was we brought professional jazz players to the NIH, and we had them memorize this piece of music on the left, the lower one, which is what you heard me playing. Then we had them improvise to the same exact chord changes. And if you could hit that lower right sound icon, that's an example of what was recorded in the scanner.

[Music] [Applause] [Music] [Applause] [Music] [Applause] And so in the end, you know, it's not the most natural environment, but they're able to play real music. And you know I've listened to that solo 200 times, and I still like it. The musicians were comfortable in the end.

So we first measured the number of notes. Were they, in fact, just playing a lot more notes when they were improvising? That was not what was going on. Then we looked at the brain activity. I'm going to try to condense this for you. These are contrast maps that are showing subtractions between what changes when you’re improvising versus when you’re doing something memorized. In red, the area that’s active is in the prefrontal cortex, frontal lobe of the brain, and in blue, this area that was deactivated.

So we had this focal area called the medial prefrontal cortex that went way up in activity. We had this broad patch of area called the lateral prefrontal cortex that went way down in activity. I'll summarize that for you here.

Now these are multifunctional areas of the brain, as I like to say. These are not the jazz areas of the brain, okay? They do a whole host of things that have to do with self-reflection, introspection, working memory, and so forth. Really, consciousness is seated in the frontal lobe. But we have this combination of an area that’s thought to be involved in self-monitoring turning off, and this area that’s thought to be autobiographical or self-expressive turning on.

We think, at least in this preliminary, you know, it's one study—it’s probably wrong—but it's one study. We think that at least a reasonable hypothesis is that to be creative, you have to have this weird dissociation in your frontal lobe. One area turns on, and a big area shuts off, so that you’re not inhibited, so that you’re willing to make mistakes, so that you’re not constantly shutting down all of these new generative impulses.

Now, a lot of people know that music is not always a solo activity; sometimes it’s done communicatively. And so the next question was: What happens when musicians are trading back and forth? Something called trading fours, which is something that they do normally in a jazz experiment. So this is a 12-bar blues, and I've broken it down into four-bar groups here, so you would know how you would trade.

Now what we did was we brought a musician into the scanner the same way, had them memorize this melody, and then had another musician out in the control room trading back and forth interactively. So this is musician Mike Pope, one of the world's best bassists and a fantastic pianist. So he’s now playing the piece that you just saw, just a little better than I wrote it.

Come on in, Mike. “Nothing’s in my pocket.” Okay. You have to have the right attitude to agree to it. It’s kind of fun, actually. And so now we’re playing back and forth. He’s in there; you can see his legs up there, and then I’m in the control room here; we’re playing back and forth.

[Music] Okay, so this is a pretty good representation of what it's like. And it’s good that it’s not too quick. You know, the fact that we do it over and over again lets you acclimate, you know, to your surroundings. So the hardest thing for me was the kinesthetic thing, you know, of just, you know, looking at my hands through two mirrors, lying on my back, and not able to move at all except my right hand. You know, that was a challenge, but again, you know, there were moments—for sure, there were moments of real honest-to-God musical interplay for sure.

So at this point, I’ll take a few moments, and so what you’re seeing here—and I’m doing a cardinal sin in science, which is just to show you preliminary data—okay, this is one subject’s data. This is, in fact, Mike Pope’s data. So what am I showing you here? When he was trading fours with me improvising versus memorizing, his language areas lit up, his Broca's area, which is the inferior frontal gyrus on the left. He actually had it also homologous on the right.

Now, this is an area thought to be involved in expressive communication. This whole notion that music is a language—well, maybe there's a neurologic basis to it, in fact, after all. And we can see it when two musicians are having a musical conversation.

And so we've done, actually, this on eight subjects now, and we're just getting all the data together. So hopefully, we’ll have something to say about it meaningfully. Now, when I think about improvisation and the language, well, what's next? Rap, of course! Freestyle!

And so I’ve always been fascinated by freestyle, and let’s go ahead and play this video.

[Music] "Here I see symmetry. I broke down and ain't the talking about late." So there's a lot of analogy between what takes place in freestyle rap and jazz. There are, in fact, a lot of correlates between the two forms of music, I think, in different time periods. In a lot of ways, rap serves the same social function that jazz used to serve.

So how do you study rap scientifically? My colleagues kind of think I'm crazy, but I think it’s very viable. And so this is what you do: You have a freestyle artist come in and memorize a rap that you write for them that they've never heard before, and then you have them freestyle. So I told my lab members that I would rap for Ted, and they said, “No, you won’t.” So, and then I thought, “No, I guess I'm not.”

But here's the thing: With this big screen, you can all rap with me, okay? So what we had him do was memorize this lower left sound icon. Please, this is the control condition.

Okay, this is what they memorized: "Memory, thump thump of the beat in a known repeat. Rhythm and rhyme make me complete. The climb is sublime when I'm on the mic spitting rhymes that hit you like a lightning strike. I search, I search for the truth in this eternal quest. My passion's not fashion; you can see how I'm dressed. Psychopathic words in my head appear; I whisper these lyrics only I can hear. The art of discovering that which is hovering inside the mind of those unconfined. All these words keep pouring out like rain; I need a mad scientist to check my brain."

Stop! I guarantee you that will never happen again. So now, what's great about these freestylers? They will get cued different words; they don't know what's coming, but they'll hear something off the cuff. Go ahead and hit that right sound icon; they're going to be cued these three square words like “not” and “head.” Freestyle doesn’t know what's coming.

Like “extraterrestrial,” “over my head,” see if I can still listen, spitting off the top. "I te children in the back of the class about apocalyptic stuff. Not really; I got to keep it simple." So again, it’s an incredible thing that’s taking place. He’s doing something that neurologically is remarkable, whether or not you like the music is irrelevant. Creatively speaking, it’s just a phenomenal thing.

This is a short video of how we actually do this in a scanner. We're here with Emanuel, who’s about—it was recorded in the scanner, by the way. That's Emanuel in the scanner. It's a tough beat; you just memorized a rhyme for us.

[Music] "I search for the truth in this eternal quest; my passion's not fashion; you can see how I'm dressed."

So I'm going to stop that there. So what do we see in his brain? Well, this is actually for rapper's brains. What we see, we do see language areas lighting up, but then eyes closed when you are freestyling versus memorizing. You’ve got major visual areas lighting up; you’ve got major cerebellar activity, which is involved in motor coordination. You have heightened brain activity when you’re doing a comparable task when one task is creative and the other task is memorized. It’s very clear, but I think it’s kind of cool.

And so just to conclude, we’ve got a lot of questions to ask, and like I said, we’ll ask questions here, not answer them. But we want to get at the root of what is creative genius neurologically. I think with these methods, we're getting close to being there. And I think hopefully in the next 10, 20 years, you'll actually see real meaningful studies that say art—and you know, science has to catch up to art, and maybe we're starting now to get there.

And so I want to thank you for your time.

More Articles

View All
The oil wars: How America's energy obsession wrecked the Middle East | Eugene Gholz | Big Think
So for a long time, oil has played a special role in American foreign policy and military strategy. Oil is a uniquely important commodity in global affairs. It’s an input to everything in our modern way of life; it’s very important for protecting our pros…
Cloud-Brained Humanoid Robots Are Right around the Corner | Big Think
The robots of the cartoons and movies from the 1970s are going to be the reality of the 2020s. And there are two real drivers behind this. First is mapping belief space. Historically, it’s been really difficult to be able to instruct robots to do things l…
The Tenth Amendment | The National Constitution Center | US government and civics | Khan Academy
Hi, this is Kim from Khan Academy, and today I’m learning about the Tenth Amendment to the Constitution, the last amendment in the Bill of Rights. The Bill of Rights was added in order to calm some of the fears held by those who felt that the new, stronge…
Example: Comparing distributions | AP Statistics | Khan Academy
What we’re going to do in this video is start to compare distributions. So for example, here we have two distributions that show the various temperatures different cities get during the month of January. This is the distribution for Portland; for example,…
The rights you might not realize you have - Shannon Odell
Since 2015, an unprecedented movement has been sweeping courts around the world. Groups of young plaintiffs, some as young as 7, are suing their governments for their inaction on tackling climate change. For example, in the winter of 2018, 25 young Colomb…
How to get ahead of 99% of people
If you want an extraordinary life, you can’t live like an ordinary person. Matt Graham said, “the people that criticize you for being exceptional are those that have already accepted the fact that they’re going to be mediocre.” A YouTuber with a million s…